Given the many health and environmental benefits of switching to a plant-based diet, there is a growing interest throughout the plant-based movement in how we might more effectively encourage behavior change in others. To successfully and quickly address the global crises related to animal food consumption, all of us—policymakers, entrepreneurs, researchers, and every individual consumer in between—must better promote the strategies that work. Equally, it would be useful to know which strategies are counterproductive so we can avoid them.
We might expect our food choices to affect the choices of those
around us and vice versa. But do we appreciate how significant this
effect can be? Are some people’s dietary choices more susceptible to
social influences than others? Is there a limit to that influence?
These are just a few of the questions researchers set out to answer
in a study published in 2018 in the journal Appetite.[1] For the
better part of a year, the researchers sneakily recorded what people
ordered for lunch at a Vancouver cafe restaurant where customers
queued and in which they could choose between comparable vegetarian
and meat options (e.g., a stew with a meat base versus a lentil
base). After collecting information on what people ordered, the
researchers surveyed those who consented to be part of the study,
asking questions about those individuals’ dietary habits, their
relationship to the person in the line ahead of them, and whether
they might have been aware of social forces at work in their
decision-making process.
The primary question the researchers wanted to answer was whether
people’s orders were influenced by the orders of those in front of
them. What they found might surprise you. And it could improve our
appreciation for what it means to be a good role model. But before
we get into the details, let’s define some critical terms.
What Is Modeling?
As defined by the researchers, modeling, in the context of food, is
“the phenomenon whereby people mimic the food intake or choice of
another person.”[1]
Notice that choice is only half of the modeling equation. Research
on intake, the other half, is already more established. A 2015
meta-analysis of 38 articles found a significant modeling effect for
food intake: “Participants ate more when their companion ate more,
and ate less when their companion ate less.”[2] Interestingly, the
modeling effect was greater for women than men. It was also greater
as an inhibiting effect than as an augmenting effect. In other
words, although people model food intake when the outcome is eating
more, they are apparently even more likely to model food intake in
the opposite direction—when their companion eats less. This makes
sense given that in most social contexts, not overeating is an
injunctive norm, especially for women (injunctive norms are those
that people perceive as being approved or not approved; they are
what we think we ought to do). It seems reasonable that this
injunctive norm might reinforce the inhibitory modeling effect.
Food choice modeling can be trickier to study, particularly with
entrees.[1] In more rigorously controlled studies, participants may
be especially susceptible to social influence because of the
heightened uncertainty of finding themselves in an unfamiliar
environment and knowing they are being studied. Observational
studies, like the 2018 study, can capture behavior in a more natural
environment, but it is difficult to rule out other influences on
food choice or to generalize the findings to the broader population.
Vegetarian or Meat?
Now, getting back to the lunchtime cafe at the University of British
Columbia, Vancouver.[1] After starting with 269 consenting
participants and excluding those who knew the person in line ahead
of them or those who were already vegetarian or vegan, the
researchers were left with 174 participants.
The headline finding was that individual orders matched the person
in front of them at a rate significantly greater than would be
expected by chance. In other words, it seems that customers were
indeed modeling the food choices of those in front of them.
“Following a prior meat-based order, the proportions of meat-based
orders increased from 73% to 82%,” exceeding the rate one would
expect from chance alone. What is encouraging is that the modeling
effect seemed even greater for vegetarian orders: “Following a
vegetarian order, the vegetarian orders increased from 27% to 47%.”
Do We Know the Forces That Move Us?
What we found most fascinating about the study is that more than
three-fourths of the participants claimed to have not been
influenced by the prior order. Still, the modeling effect was
significant even among that group. This suggests that modeling may
occur beyond our awareness, a notion well-supported by the wider
scientific literature on social influence.[3] Generally, we
underestimate external influences on our choices, perhaps because we
desire to feel control over our destinies. One exception is when
admitting to external influences supports self-serving biases. For
example, women in a pasta-feeding study were more willing to cite
external influences (portion size) as an “excuse” for overeating.[4]
While participants may have been influenced “simply [by] witnessing
the prior order being prepared,” the researchers note that the meal
choices were all in an open display, so visual prominence is
unlikely to be the sole factor driving order-matching. Other limits
of the study include its relatively small sample size and—I suspect
more importantly—its location on a university campus. It is
impossible to know whether an equally robust modeling effect might
occur in different settings with populations not predominantly made
up of students, who may be more susceptible to social influence or
more open to shifting toward plant-based diets.
The Power of Being a Good Role Model
Given the many health and environmental benefits of switching to a
plant-based diet, there is a growing interest throughout the
plant-based movement in how we might more effectively encourage
behavior change in others. To successfully and quickly address the
global crises related to animal food consumption, all of
us—policymakers, entrepreneurs, researchers, and every individual
consumer in between—must better promote the strategies that work.
Equally, it would be useful to know which strategies are
counterproductive so we can avoid them.
This is easier said than done. Because we are dealing with
relatively new areas of study, there remain many more questions than
answers. The effectiveness of confronting people with information is
unclear. So much depends on the type of information you share and
the circumstances in which you share it. In some cases, even
seemingly harmless approaches might backfire. I’m sure many of you
can think of a few examples in which trying to start a conversation
about a plant-based lifestyle might do more to shut down interest
than to inspire change.
To further complicate the issue, social norms do not always work as
expected. In a study published earlier this year, researchers found
small and statistically insignificant effects of social norms on
first-year students “confronted with different statements about the
diets of students already enrolled and studying at the
university.”[5] Notably, they found that female students were much
more responsive to this explicit exposure to social norms than their
male counterparts, especially when their food choices could be
observed by others. In another study, researchers found that norm
following is likelier to occur when norms are conveyed implicitly
and the circumstances do not feel unfamiliar; however, participants
who had low intentions to follow a vegetarian diet from the
beginning of a study “[exhibited] reactance against an explicit
vegetarian norm in an unfamiliar context [emphasis added].”[6] That
is, those who were not interested in the vegetarian diet in the
first place became even less likely to choose vegetarian options
when confronted with an explicit norm. Intuitively, this makes
sense. We have probably all met someone, at some point, who
exhibited this kind of stubbornness.
One thing that cannot backfire, though, is being a positive role
model. While more research in this area is needed, we now have at
least some compelling evidence that our example alone can inspire
others to choose healthier options, whether we know it or not—even
whether they know it or not! So make sure to continue treating
yourself well: eat wholesome foods, prioritize learning, exercise
regularly, and cultivate a healthy social life. You never know who
might be watching.
References