Stephen Kaufman, M.D., Christian Vegetarian Association (CVA)
Who Are the Animals People Abuse?
Last week, I suggested that humans hold in contempt those animals people
abuse and kill. Why should this be? If humans felt perfectly entitled to
harm nonhumans, then there would be no need to also hold these nonhumans in
contempt. Most people don’t have contempt for a tree they might cut down,
because most don’t believe that trees have feelings that can be harmed in a
moral sense. I think contempt toward nonhumans reflects humans’ ambivalence
about abusing them, and those who mistreat animals recognize that doing so
raises moral concerns. Animal abuse becomes justified if animals “deserve”
such treatment.
The specific charges humans levy against animals, I think, tells us little
about animals but much about humans. People often claim that nonhumans have
unattractive attributes and use animal names as insults against humans. For
example, calling a person a “pig” indicates that they are gluttonous, a
stupid person is a “turkey,” a grotesquely fat person is a “cow,” and a
fearful person is “chicken.” These attributes do not define the named
animals, whose physical and behavioral attributes are appropriate for their
species and are conducive to survival.
Perhaps if we exposed the lies about the nature of God’s animals we could
awaken moral sensibilities that would help prevent animal abuse. I will
reflect on this more next essay.
Many people regard nonhumans as instinctive, machine-like creations. In
truth, they show feelings and desires that are obvious to anyone paying
attention. They also show intelligence, concern for others, and even moral
behavior.
For example, a series of laboratory studies (which I should note was
ethically dubious) illustrated moral character among monkeys as well as an
important degree of abstract thinking ability. In one study, rhesus monkeys,
upon pulling one of two chains to get food, observed through a one-way
mirror a second rhesus monkey receiving a simultaneous electric shock. One
chain caused a fellow monkey to receive an electric shock, and the other did
not. Ten of fifteen monkeys preferred the non-shock chain, and two monkeys
did not pull either chain, preferring instead to go without food for 5 days
and 12 days. Self-starvation was more likely among monkeys who had
previously received electric shocks themselves. (See Masserman, Jules H.,
Stanley Wechkin, and William Terris. 1964. “ ‘Altruistic’ behavior in rhesus
monkeys.” American Journal of Psychiatry vol. 121, pp. 584-585.)
Similarly, in the documentary People of the Forest, an adult male chimpanzee
watched over and protected an unrelated, crippled, adolescent chimpanzee
from the torment of other adolescent chimpanzees. (See Van Lawick, Hugo,
director. 1991. People of the Forest, Discovery Channel Video.)
Next essay, I will reflect on intelligence among nonhumans.
Go on to: Intelligence and Moral Rights
Return to:
Reflection on the Lectionary, Table of Contents